digplanet beta 1: Athena
Share digplanet:

Agriculture

Applied sciences

Arts

Belief

Business

Chronology

Culture

Education

Environment

Geography

Health

History

Humanities

Language

Law

Life

Mathematics

Nature

People

Politics

Science

Society

Technology

Episodic memory is the memory of autobiographical events (times, places, associated emotions, and other contextual who, what, when, where, why knowledge) that can be explicitly stated. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at a particular time and place. For example, if you remember the party on your 6th birthday, this is an episodic memory. They allow you to figuratively travel back in time to remember the event that took place at that particular time and place.[1]

Semantic and episodic memory together make up the category of declarative memory, which is one of the two major divisions of memory – the other is implicit memory.[2] The term "episodic memory" was coined by Endel Tulving in 1972. He was referring to the distinction between knowing and remembering. Knowing is more factual (semantic) whereas remembering is a feeling that is located in the past (episodic).[3]

Tulving has seminally defined three key properties of episodic memory recollection. These are: a subjective sense of time (or mental time travel), connection to the self, and autonoetic consciousness. Autonoetic consciousness refers to a special kind of consciousness that accompanies the act of remembering which enables an individual to be aware of the self in a subjective time. Aside from Tulving, others named the important aspects of recollection which includes visual imagery, narrative structure, retrieval of semantic information and the feelings of familiarity.[4]

Events that are recorded into episodic memory may trigger episodic learning, i.e. a change in behavior that occurs as a result of an event.[5][6] For example, a fear of dogs after being bitten by a dog is a result of episodic learning.

One of the main components of episodic memory is the process of recollection. Recollection is a process that elicits the retrieval of contextual information pertaining to a specific event or experience that has occurred.

Nine properties of episodic memory[edit]

There are essentially nine properties of episodic memory that collectively distinguish it from other types of memory. Other types of memory may exhibit a few of these properties, but only episodic memory has all nine:[7]

  1. Contain summary records of sensory-perceptual-conceptual-affective processing.
  2. Retain patterns of activation/inhibition over long periods.
  3. Often represented in the form of (visual) images.
  4. They always have a perspective (field or observer).
  5. Represent short time slices of experience.
  6. They are represented on a temporal dimension roughly in order of occurrence.
  7. They are subject to rapid forgetting.
  8. They make autobiographical remembering specific.
  9. They are recollectively experienced when accessed.

Cognitive neuroscience[edit]

The formation of new episodic memories requires the medial temporal lobe, a structure that includes the hippocampus. Without the medial temporal lobe, one is able to form new procedural memories (such as playing the piano) but cannot remember the events during which they happened (See the hippocampus and memory).

The prefrontal cortex (and in particular the left hemisphere) is also involved in the formation of new episodic memories (also known as episodic encoding). Patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex can learn new information, but tend to do so in a disordered fashion. For example, they might show normal recognition of an object they had seen in the past, but fail to recollect when or where it had been viewed.[8] Some researchers believe that the prefrontal cortex helps organize information for more efficient storage, drawing upon its role in executive function. Others believe that the prefrontal cortex underlies semantic strategies which enhance encoding, such as thinking about the meaning of the study material or rehearsing it in working memory.[9]

Researchers do not agree about how long episodic memories are stored in the hippocampus. Some researchers believe that episodic memories always rely on the hippocampus. Others believe the hippocampus only stores episodic memories for a short time, after which the memories are consolidated to the neocortex. The latter view is strengthened by recent evidence that neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus may ease the removal of old memories and increase the efficiency of forming new memories.[10]

Relationship to semantic memory[edit]

Endel Tulving originally described episodic memory as a record of a person’s experience that held temporally dated information and spatio-temporal relations.[11] A feature of episodic memory that Tulving later elaborates on is that it allows an agent to imagine traveling back in time.[12] A current situation may cue retrieval of a previous episode, so that context that colours the previous episode is experienced at the immediate moment. The agent is provided with a means of associating previous feelings with current situations. Semantic memory, on the other hand, is a structured record of facts, concepts and skills that we have acquired. Semantic information is derived from accumulated episodic memory. Episodic memory can be thought of as a "map" that ties together items in semantic memory. For example, all encounters with how a "dog" looks and sounds will make up the semantic representation of that word. All episodic memories concerning your dog will then reference this single semantic representation of "dog" and, likewise, all new experiences with your dog will modify your single semantic representation of your dog.

Together, semantic and episodic memory make up our declarative memory.[13] They each represent different parts of context to form a complete picture. As such, something that affects episodic memory can also affect semantic memory. For example, anterograde amnesia, from damage of the medial temporal lobe, is an impairment of declarative memory that affects both episodic and semantic memory operations.[14] Originally, Tulving proposed that episodic and semantic memory were separate systems that competed with each other in retrieval. However, this theory was rejected when Howard and Kahana completed experiments on latent semantic analysis (LSA) that supported the opposite. Instead of an increase in semantic similarity when there was a decrease in the strength of temporal associations, the two worked together so semantic cues on retrieval were strongest when episodic cues were strong as well.[15]

Age differences[edit]

Activation of specific brain areas (mostly the hippocampus) seems to be different between younger and older people upon episodic memory retrieval.[16] Older people tend to activate both left and right hippocampus, while younger people activate only the left one. For more information, see memory and aging.

Relationship to emotion[edit]

The relationship between emotion and memory is complex, but generally, emotion tends to increase the likelihood that an event will be remembered later and that it will be remembered vividly. Flashbulb memory is one example of this. An example of this would be an experience such as a close family member dying or the Christmas that you got the exact toy you wanted as a kid. The experience holds so much emotional significance that it is encoded as an extremely vivid, almost picture-perfect memory.

Pharmacological enhancement[edit]

In healthy adults, longterm visual episodic memory can be enhanced specifically[17] through administration of the Acetylcholine esterase inhibitor Donepezil, whereas verbal episodic memory can be improved in persons with the val/val genotype of the val158met polymorphism through administration of the CNS penetrant specific catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitor Tolcapone.[18] Furthermore, episodic memory is enhanced through AZD3480, a selective agonist at the neuronal alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptor, which is developed by the company Targacept.[19] Currently, there are several other products developed by several companies—including new catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitors with fewer side effects—that aim for improving episodic memory. A recent placebo controlled study found that DHEA, which is a functional cortisol antagonist, improves episodic memory in healthy young men (Alhaj et al. 2006).[20]

Damage[edit]

  • Based on a review of behavioral studies, it is suggested that there may be selective damage to the limbic-prefrontal episodic memory system in some people with autism.[21] Another study points to evidence of autistic deficits in the episodic or self-conscious memory of personally experienced events (Joseph et al., 2003).
  • The label "amnesia" is most often given to patients with deficits in episodic memory.
  • Alzheimer's disease tends to damage the hippocampus before other brain areas.
  • A rare type of shellfish poisoning called amnesic shellfish poisoning or "ASP" quite effectively and irreversibly damages the hippocampus, rendering one amnesic.
  • Korsakoff's syndrome is caused by thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency, a form of malnutrition which can be precipitated by overconsumption of alcohol compared to foods.
  • An acute cortisol level (by injection) has been found to significantly inhibit the recall of autobiographical memories[22] which may contribute to memory deficits found in depression.
  • The use of some illicit drugs such as MDMA ("Ecstasy") has been associated with persistent deficits in episodic memory.[23][24]

In animals[edit]

Main article: Episodic-like memory

Tulving (1983) proposed that to meet the criteria of episodic memory, evidence of conscious recollection must be provided. Demonstrating episodic memory in the absence of language, and thus in non-human animals, is impossible, because there are no agreed upon non-linguistic behavioral indicators of conscious experience (Griffiths et al., 1999).

This idea was first challenged by Clayton and Dickinson in their work with the Western Scrub Jay (Aphelocoma californica). They were able to demonstrate that these birds may possess an episodic-like memory system as they found that they remember where they cached different food types, and discriminately recovered them depending on the perishability of the item and time that elapsed since caching. Thus, scrub-jays appear to remember the "what-where-and-when" of specific past caching events. The authors argued that such performance meets the behavioral criteria for episodic memory, but referred to the ability as "episodic-like" memory because the study did not address the phenomenological aspects of episodic memory.

After a study done by the University of Edinburgh (2006), hummingbirds were the first animal to demonstrate two of the aspects of episodic memory— the ability to recall where certain flowers were located and how recently they were visited. Other studies have demonstrated this type of memory in different animal species, such as rats, honey bees, and primates.

To show that animals do encode and retrieve past experiences relies on the circuitry of the medial temporal lobe, a structure including the hippocampus. Animal lesion studies have provided significant findings related to the importance of particular brain structures in episodic-like memory. For example, hippocampal lesions have severely impacted all three components (what, where, and when) in animals, suggesting that the hippocampus is responsible for detecting novel events, stimuli, and places when forming new memories and on retrieving that information later on.

Despite similar neural areas and evidence from experiments, some scholars remain cautious about comparisons to human episodic memory (Suddendorf & Busby, 2003). Purported episodic-like memory often seems fixed to a particular domain or could be explained in terms of procedural or semantic memory. The problem may be better tractable by studying episodic memory's adaptive counterpart: the capacity to flexibly imagine future events. However, a recent experiment addressed one of Suddendorf and Busby (2003)'s specific criticisms (the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis, which states that nonhuman animals can only take actions based on immediate needs, as opposed to future needs). Correia and colleagues demonstrated [citation needed] that Western scrub-jays can selectively cache different types of foods depending on which type of food they will desire at a future time, offering strong evidence against the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis by demonstrating that scrub-jays can flexibly adjust their behavior based on past experience of desiring a particular food.

Autobiographical memory[edit]

An autobiographical memory is a personal representation of general or specific events and personal facts. Autobiographical memory also refers to memory of a person's history. An individual does not remember exactly everything that has happened in one's past. Memory is constructive, where previous experience affects how we remember events and what we end up recalling from memory. Autobiographical memory is constructive and reconstructed as an evolving process of history. A person's autobiographical memory is fairly reliable; although, the reliability of autobiographical memories is questionable because of memory distortions.

Autobiographical memories can differ for special periods of life. People recall few personal events from the first years of their lives. The loss of these first events is called childhood or infantile amnesia. People tend to recall many personal events from adolescence and early adulthood. This effect is called the reminiscence bump. Finally, people recall many personal events from the last few years. This is called the recency effect. For adolescents and young adults the reminiscence bump and the recency effect coincide.

It is known that autobiographical memories initially are stored as episodic memories, but it is currently unknown if autobiographical memories are the same as episodic memories or if the autobiographical memories become converted to semantic memories with time.

Types[edit]

  • Specific events
    • When you first set foot in the ocean.
  • General events
    • What it feels like stepping into the ocean in general. This is a memory of what a personal event is generally like. It might be based on the memories of having stepped in the ocean, many times during the years.
  • Personal facts
    • "Who was the Prime Minister of Italy when I was born?"
  • Flashbulb memories
    • Flashbulb memories are critical autobiographical memories about a major event. Some flashbulb memories are shared within a social group:
"The assassination of John Kennedy?"
"The assassination of Martin Luther King, Jr.?"
"The Challenger explosion?"
"The verdict in the O.J. Simpson trial?"
"When you learned that Princess Diana had died?"
"When you heard about 9/11?"

Neural network models[edit]

Episodic memories can be stored in autoassociative neural networks (e.g., a Hopfield network) if the stored representation includes information on the spatiotemporal context in which an item was studied.

Notes[edit]

  1. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Gilbert, Daniel T., and Wegner, Daniel M. "Semantic and episodic memory". Psychology; Second Edition. New York: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 240-241. Print.
  2. ^ Tulving E (1984). "Precis of Elements of Episodic Memory". Behavioural and Brain Sciences 7 (2): 223–68. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0004440X. 
  3. ^ [1] from an article in "Current Biology"
  4. ^ [2] Hassabis D and Maguire EA. 2007. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences (Regul Ed ) 11(7):299-306.
  5. ^ Terry, W. S. (2006). Learning and Memory: Basic principles, processes, and procedures. Boston: Pearson Education, Inc.
  6. ^ Baars, B. J. & Gage, N. M. (2007). Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. London: Elsevier Ltd.
  7. ^ [3] Conway M. A. (2009). Episodic Memory. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2305-2306
  8. ^ Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Squire LR (1989). "Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions". Neuropsychologia 27 (8): 1043–56. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90184-X. PMID 2797412. 
  9. ^ Gabrieli JD, Poldrack RA, Desmond JE (February 1998). "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95 (3): 906–13. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.906. PMC 33815. PMID 9448258. 
  10. ^ Deisseroth K, Singla S, Toda H, Monje M, Palmer TD, Malenka RC (May 2004). "Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells". Neuron 42 (4): 535–52. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00266-1. PMID 15157417. 
  11. ^ Tulving, Endel (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. New York: Oxford University Press. 
  12. ^ Tulving, Endel (2002). "Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain". Annual Review of Psychology 53: 1–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. PMID 11752477. 
  13. ^ Tulving, Endel; Schacter, D.L. (19 January 1990). "Priming and Human Memory Systems". Science. 4940 247 (4940): 301–6. doi:10.1126/science.2296719. PMID 2296719. 
  14. ^ Tulving, Endel; Hans J. Markowitsch (7 December 1998). "Episodic and Declarative Memory: role of the hippocampus". Hippocampus 8 (3): 198–204. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G. PMID 9662134. 
  15. ^ Howard, M.W.; Kahana, M.J. (2002). "When does semantic similarity help episodic retrieval". Journal of Memory and Language 46: 85–96. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2798. 
  16. ^ Maguire EA, Frith CD (July 2003). "Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval". Brain 126 (Pt 7): 1511–23. doi:10.1093/brain/awg157. PMID 12805116. 
  17. ^ Grön G, Kirstein M, Thielscher A, Riepe MW, Spitzer M (October 2005). "Cholinergic enhancement of episodic memory in healthy young adults". Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 182 (1): 170–9. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0043-2. PMID 16021483. 
  18. ^ Apud JA, Mattay V, Chen J et al. (May 2007). "Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects". Neuropsychopharmacology 32 (5): 1011–20. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301227. PMID 17063156. 
  19. ^ Dunbar G, Boeijinga PH, Demazières A et al. (May 2007). "Effects of TC-1734 (AZD3480), a selective neuronal nicotinic receptor agonist, on cognitive performance and the EEG of young healthy male volunteers". Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 191 (4): 919–29. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0675-x. PMID 17225162. 
  20. ^ Alhaj HA, Massey AE, McAllister-Williams RH (November 2006). "Effects of DHEA administration on episodic memory, cortisol and mood in healthy young men: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study". Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 188 (4): 541–51. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0136-y. PMID 16231168. 
  21. ^ Ben Shalom D (2003). "Memory in autism: review and synthesis". Cortex 39 (4–5): 1129–38. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70881-5. PMID 14584570. 
  22. ^ Buss C, Wolf OT, Witt J, Hellhammer DH (September 2004). "Autobiographic memory impairment following acute cortisol administration". Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (8): 1093–6. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.09.006. PMID 15219661. 
  23. ^ Parrott AC, Lees A, Garnham NJ, Jones M, Wesnes K (1998). "Cognitive performance in recreational users of MDMA of 'ecstasy': evidence for memory deficits". Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 12 (1): 79–83. doi:10.1177/026988119801200110. PMID 9584971. 
  24. ^ Morgan MJ (January 1999). "Memory deficits associated with recreational use of "ecstasy" (MDMA)". Psychopharmacology 141 (1): 30–6. doi:10.1007/s002130050803. PMID 9952062. 

References[edit]

  • Deisseroth K, Singla S, Toda H, Monje M, Palmer TD, Malenka RC (2004). "Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells". Neuron 42 (4): 535–52. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00266-1. PMID 15157417. 
  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory, (pp. 381–403). New York: Academic Press.
  • Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Tulving E (2002). "Episodic memory: from mind to brain". Annual review of psychology 53: 1–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. PMID 11752477. 
  • Griffiths D, Dickinson A, Clayton N (1999). "Episodic memory: what can animals remember about their past?". Trends in cognitive sciences 3 (2): 74–80. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01272-8. PMID 10234230. 
  • Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998). "Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays". Nature 395 (6699): 272–4. doi:10.1038/26216. PMID 9751053. 
  • Kart-Teke E, De Souza Silva MA, Huston JP, Dere E (2006). "Wistar rats show episodic-like memory for unique experiences". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 85 (2): 173–82. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2005.10.002. PMID 16290193. 
  • Eacott MJ, Easton A, Zinkivskay A (2005). "Recollection in an episodic-like memory task in the rat". Learn. Mem. 12 (3): 221–3. doi:10.1101/lm.92505. PMID 15897259. 
  • Suddendorf T, Busby J (2003). "Mental time travel in animals?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (9): 391–396. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00187-6. PMID 12963469. 
  • Suddendorf T (2006). "Foresight and evolution of the human mind". Science 312 (5776): 1006–7. doi:10.1126/science.1129217. PMID 16709773. 
  • Ghetti, S. Lee, J. (2010). Children's Episodic Memory. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. pp. 365–373.

External links[edit]


Original courtesy of Wikipedia: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Episodic_memory — Please support Wikipedia.
This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia. A portion of the proceeds from advertising on Digplanet goes to supporting Wikipedia.
15581 videos foundNext > 

Episodic Memory: Understanding How Memories are Made in Kids

Nora Newcombe, professor of psychology and director of Temple Infant and Children Laboratory, explains more about her research on episodic memory. Learn more...

The Hippocampus and episodic memory

A video describing the anatomy of the Hippocampus, its functions, its circuity and possible function in episodic memory.

Chapter 7 - Episodic and Semantic Memory

Chapter 7 - Episodic and Semantic Memory.

Psychology 1- Episodic, Semantic, Procedural, Memory

Psychology 1 extra credit on episodic, semantic, procedural, memory for Professor Altman.

Episodic Memory

Cengage Psychology Get Psyched Video Contest Entry Footage Provided By: Grandma and Grandpa Radwan Music and Additional Footage By: Matthew Radwan and Alexan...

Episodic Memory-Properties that Define the Memory

http://episodicmemory.net- An episodic memory does not take very long to form and that has ensured people keep away from making serious and costly mistakes m...

Episodic Memory

Describes facts on episodic memory.

51 First Dates: Episodic Memory Loss (UNC Psyc 222)

Understanding the Link Between Context and Episodic Memory

Michael Kahana (University of Pennsylvania) uses mathematical modeling and computational techniques to study human memory. Episodic memory refers to memory f...

Episodic Memory

Song: Episodic Memory Artist: Question the Architect Album: The Demonstration Released: 2012 Facebook: https://www.facebook.com/QuestionTheArchitect Free Dow...

15581 videos foundNext > 

47 news items

 
The Epoch Times
Wed, 27 Aug 2014 09:41:15 -0700

By linking a memory to a where we were at that time, and integrating many stimuli together, helps us remember something particularly important. They call this process episodic memory formation: the locking of ideas and objects to a single place and time.
 
Forbes
Mon, 04 Aug 2014 05:00:11 -0700

The interviews evaluated three areas of cognitive performance: (1) episodic memory (the ability to recall words); numeracy (the ability to reason with basic mathematical concepts); and category fluency (the ability to name examples from categories ...
 
Scientific American (blog)
Wed, 20 Aug 2014 13:21:26 -0700

Episodic Memory If you lived forever, would you remember everything that happened to you? The answer is no. Memory decays exponentially over time. In the Figure below, the horizontal axis represents time, starting from the present on the right, going ...

Daily Digest

Daily Digest
Tue, 12 Aug 2014 19:26:15 -0700

Scientists call this episodic memory formation. They suggest it is possible that by connecting several aspects of an event to a single location and time frame a more complete memory is retained and is able to be recalled at a later date. A person ...
 
Wired.co.uk
Wed, 30 Jul 2014 03:12:07 -0700

Researchers are particularly interested to figure out if animals have what's known as episodic memory, or the ability to recall contextual information about past events, such as what was happening, or where and when it occurred. Stewart described this ...
 
Eureka! Science News
Thu, 31 Jul 2014 01:57:40 -0700

"We haven't previously been aware of this intermediate form of memory, which combines semantic knowledge with autobiographical, or 'episodic' memory. "The hope is that advanced methods could be developed to test this newly discovered intermediate ...
 
The Australian
Mon, 28 Jul 2014 17:17:06 -0700

“However, the findings from this study suggest that if women and men had equal levels of education, then we should expect a female advantage in episodic memory, a male advantage in numeracy, and no gender differences in category fluency, such as ...

Laboratory Equipment

Laboratory Equipment
Wed, 20 Aug 2014 04:15:00 -0700

The study used the Quality of Life-AD scale, the Cambridge Cognitive Examination of the Elderly as well as a broad range of established neuropsychological tests to assess cognitive functions such as episodic memory, naming, language comprehension, ...
Loading

Oops, we seem to be having trouble contacting Twitter

Talk About Episodic memory

You can talk about Episodic memory with people all over the world in our discussions.

Support Wikipedia

A portion of the proceeds from advertising on Digplanet goes to supporting Wikipedia. Please add your support for Wikipedia!